BMJ. 2003 July 19; 327(7407): 139–142.
A levels and intelligence as predictors of medical careers in UK doctors:
20 year prospective study
I C McManus,
professor of psychology and medical education,
1 Eleni Smithers,
medical student,
1 Philippa Partridge,
medical student,
1 A Keeling,
research assistant,
1 and
Peter R Fleming,
former assistant dean and senior lecturer in medicine, Westminster
Medical School2 1 Department of Psychology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT
2 London SW20 9DY
This article has been
cited by other articles in PMC.
Abstract
Objective To assess whether A level grades (achievement) and
intelligence (ability) predict doctors' careers.
Design Prospective cohort study with follow up after 20 years by
postal questionnaire.
Setting A UK medical school in London.
Participants 511 doctors who had entered Westminster Medical School
as clinical students between 1975 and 1982 were followed up in January
2002.
Main outcome measures Time taken to reach different career grades in
hospital or general practice, postgraduate qualifications obtained
(membership/fellowships, diplomas, higher academic degrees), number of
research publications, and measures of stress and burnout related to A level
grades and intelligence (result of AH5 intelligence test) at entry to clinical
school. General health questionnaire, Maslach burnout inventory, and
questionnaire on satisfaction with career at follow up.
Results 47 (9%) doctors were no longer on the Medical Register. They
had lower A level grades than those who were still on the register (P <
0.001). A levels also predicted performance in undergraduate training,
performance in postregistration house officer posts, and time to achieve
membership qualifications (Cox regression, P < 0.001; b=0.376, SE=0.098,
exp(b)=1.457). Intelligence did not independently predict dropping off the
register, career outcome, or other measures. A levels did not predict diploma
or higher academic qualifications, research publications, or stress or
burnout. Diplomas, higher academic degrees, and research publications did,
however, significantly correlate with personality measures.
Conclusions Results of achievement tests, in this case A level
grades, which are particularly used for selection of students in the United
Kingdom, have long term predictive validity for undergraduate and postgraduate
careers. In contrast, a test of ability or aptitude (AH5) was of little
predictive validity for subsequent medical careers.
Introduction
Selection of UK medical students depends mainly on grades achieved in
school leaving examinations, such as A
levels.1 Few long
term studies have validated such selection
measures,2 and their
theoretical underpinning is unclear. Examinations measure achievement,
accomplishment, or attainment and assess whether students have mastered an
academic subject. In contrast, measures of ability or aptitude assess
cognitive ability independently of cultural content and educational experience
and are typified by measures of intelligence (general mental
ability3) (see
www.bmj.com).
Whereas intelligence shows stability through
life,4 achievement
tests depend mainly on recent educational experience.
Although seldom articulated, three arguments underpin selection with
achievement tests:
The achievement argument—A levels ensure a minimum competence in the
sciences basic to medicine, such as chemistry and biology
The ability argument—Academic success depends mainly on intellectual
ability,
2 and
achievement tests indirectly assess intelligence. Because achievement tests
can be biased or inaccurate, due to poor schooling, absent role models, low
expectations, or inappropriate motivation, there is a case for replacing A
levels with measures of aptitude or
ability
5,6The motivation argument—A levels are effective because university
education requires not only intellectual ability but also good study skills
and motivation. High A level grades indicate both satisfactory intellectual
ability and learning style. The content of the course therefore matters less
than the fact of success.
To distinguish such positions we need to relate career outcomes to
achievement and intellectual ability. In 2002 we followed up a cohort of
clinical students who had taken a standard intelligence test when they entered
Westminster Medical School between 1975 and 1982. We had four outcome
measures:
Dropout—Whatever the problems of defining success in a medical
career, doctors not on the Medical Register are not successful as practising
doctors, albeit that non-clinicians provide much benefit to medicine and
society
Career progression—Medical careers are hierarchical. Speed of
progression and of attaining postgraduate qualifications therefore indicate
success. Although exceptions occur, doctors who take longer to reach the top
realise their potential less
Research output—Many doctors publish research and some publish a lot.
The implicit presumption is that productive research is the prerogative of the
brightest and the best (and typically is the basis for MB-PhD selection)
Stress, burnout, and satisfaction with medicine as a career—A
successful doctor is a happy doctor, with low stress and burnout and high
career satisfaction. Although less intellectually able doctors may suffer
stress due to difficulties in keeping up to date as practice changes, a more
subtle converse argument suggests that stressed doctors are those with highest
ability, day to day practice providing insufficient variation for adequate
intellectual
stimulation.
7 patient interaction.
For practical reasons we could not assess doctor-patient interaction.
Method
PRF administered the AH5 (a timed “high grade” intelligence
test8) to clinical
students entering the Westminster Medical School from 1975 to 1982. The test
has measures of verbal and reasoning ability (part I, “verbal”)
and spatial ability (part II, “spatial”). Students were informed
that the test was confidential and for research and that results would not be
available to teachers or examiners.
In 1988, PRF and ICM collated the results with date of birth, sex, A
levels, intercalated degree results, finals performance, and performance in
preregistration
posts.9 A levels
were scored as A=5 to E=1 and O/F=0, and summarised as the mean. Performance
at finals was recorded as 4=distinction, 3=pass all first time, 2=pass after
resits, 1=fail. Preregistration performance was the average consultant rating
(4=outstanding, 3=good, 2=satisfactory, 1=inadequate).
In 2001 we used the Medical Register and Directory to trace the graduates.
In January 2002 we sent a questionnaire to those on the 2001 UK Medical
Register; non-respondents were sent two reminders. The questionnaire asked
about career, qualifications, interests, and
personality.10,11 We assessed stress
with the general health questionnaire (GHQ-12) and an abbreviated Maslach
burnout inventory
(aMBI),12 with
additional questions on satisfaction with medicine (see
www.bmj.com).
Statistical analysis used SPSS 10.0 and LISREL 8.51.
Results
The mean total AH5 score (fig A,
bmj.com) of 40.4
was similar to
norms,8 as were
verbal and spatial scores (table A,
bmj.com). The mean
A level score (fig A,
bmj.com) was 4.00,
equivalent to grade BBB. AH5 score and A level grade were correlated (Pearson
r=0.285, P < 0.001; fig B,
bmj.com).
Dropouts from Medical Register—All 511 students registered
with the General Medical Council, but only 464 were on the 2001 Medical
Register. The 47 doctors who left the register (a mean of 11.1 years after
qualifying; SD 5.9; range 2-23) had lower A level grades but not lower AH5
scores (table A,
bmj.com); see
www.bmj.com for ROC analysis. Two doctors subsequently returned to the register. Of the
remainder, three had died, contact details were available for 35, and no
information was available for seven.
Questionnaire response—Of the 464 doctors on the register,
349 (73%) replied to the questionnaire. Non-respondents had lower AH5 scores
but did not have different A levels results (table A,
bmj.com).
Career choice and career progression—Of 332 doctors for whom
we had usable information, 173 worked in hospital (149 were consultants) and
131 in general practice (116 were principals). Of the remainder, four were not
working, five had non-medical posts, and 19 had other medical posts. Hospital
doctors had higher A level grades and AH5 scores (see table A on
bmj.com), each
effect being significant after we accounted for the other (A levels: Student's
t test, t299 = 2.674, P=0.008; AH5: t299 =
2.059, P=0.040). Remaining analyses therefore took differences in speciality
into account. shows
the career progression of hospital doctors and general practitioners.
Qualifications are grouped into memberships (MRCP, FRCS, etc), diplomas (or
equivalent, often offered by Royal Colleges), and academic degrees (PhD, MD,
masters, or bachelors degree). A levels had a highly significant effect on
years to membership (table, Cox regression, P < 0.001;
), even after we accounted
for AH5 (P=0.001). AH5 had a significant simple effect on years to membership
(P=0.049) but not after we accounted for A levels (P=0.401). Other effects of
A levels and AH5 were not significant after we accounted for multiple
testing.
Careers of doctors in hospital medicine and general practice (career grade
above and acquisition of memberships, diplomas, and academic degrees
below)
Kaplan-Meier plots for percentage of doctors obtaining membership in
relation to A level grade, after taking hospital/general practice differences
into account
Structural modelling of educational achievement—We modelled
academic and professional achievement using structural equation modelling with
causal order mainly determined by temporal order, except that we regarded AH5
score before A levels. Goodness of fit was excellent (χ2=4.90,
df=8, P=0.768; GFI (goodness of fit index)=0.995; AGFI (adjusted goodness of
fit index)=0.988). Each stage predicted the subsequent stage, and A level
grade and finals performance had additional direct effects on time to
membership ().
Path model of causal associations between different educational
achievements of doctors. Coefficients represent standardised path coefficients
(β coefficients) with their associated significance levels
Research publications—In total 138 doctors (40%) had not
published any research papers, 44 (13%) had published 1-2 papers, 36 (11%) 3-5
papers, 30 (9%) 6-10 papers, 39 (11%) 11-20 papers, 18 (5%) 51-100 papers, 29
(9%) 21-50 papers, 8 (2%) had published more than 100 papers. Regression of
normal scores (ranked normal deviates; normal order statistics) showed
differences and between hospital doctors and general practitioners (P <
0.001) but no effect of A levels or AH5 score
().
Effects of mean A level grade, total AH5 score, and AH5 verbal and spatial
subscores on various outcome measures. All effects are simple effects that do
not take other variables into account; all analyses, however, take differences
in general practice/hospital ...
Stress, burnout, and satisfaction with medicine as a
career—Sixty two doctors (18%) scored ≥ 4 on the general health
questionnaire, indicating “caseness” for stress. General
practitioners scored higher than hospital doctors on measures of emotional
exhaustion, depersonalisation, and personal accomplishment in the Maslach
burnout inventory but did not differ on the general health questionnaire
(0-1-2-3 scoring) or on satisfaction with a medical career. No measure showed
any association with A level grades or AH5 score
().
Discussion
Few studies have attempted to validate the selection procedures for medical
students, although in such studies the effect size of academic measures for
postgraduate performance is
0.48.2 Despite A
levels being the basis for selection in the United
Kingdom,1 little
evaluation has taken place, and although occasional comments suggest that A
levels are “completely unpredictive” they actually predict early
dropout from medical
school.13,14 For university
degrees overall, A levels also predict degree class, dropout, and repeated
years, particularly for
science.13,14 We have shown that A
level results, which are measures of achievement, can predict time taken to
gain membership qualifications, choosing to become a general practitioner, and
leaving the register. In contrast the AH5, which measures ability, cannot
independently predict membership qualifications or dropout.
A levels therefore have validity in selection, with a validity coefficient
of about 0.3 (see
www.bmj.com),
although care should be taken in generalising the results to other
examinations in other countries. Intelligence does not predict careers, thus
rejecting the ability argument. A levels predict because they assess
achievement, and the structural model shows how past achievements predict
future achievement. Our data cannot distinguish the achievement argument and
the motivation argument, although the long term, direct effect of A levels on
membership examinations ()
suggests that motivation might be important.
Despite their predictive ability, A levels are probably not the only
predictors2 and
should not be the sole basis for
selection.15 Some
of our other outcomes were not predicted by A levels but were correlated with
measures of personality (see
www.bmj.com)
and would probably also be predicted by learning
styles.16,17 West answered Smith's
editorial question of “Why are doctors so
unhappy?”18 by suggesting that burn out because they are overqualified for a repetitious
job.7 The causes of
stress and burnout in doctors are
complex,12 but our
data suggest that excess intellectual ability is not one of them.
What is already known on this topic
There are few prospective studies of achievement tests used in student
selection, such as A levels, in relation to outcomes in medical careers
It is not clear whether A levels are useful in selection because they
assess knowledge, motivation, study habits, or ability (intelligence)
What this study adds
A level results can predict outcome in medical careers
An ability test (the AH5 intelligence test) does not predict outcome
It is not clear yet whether the predictive value of A levels results from
assessing knowledge, motivation, or study habits
Other measures such as personality are also probably important in
predicting outcome doctors
Supplementary Material
Definitions, extra tables and extra figures:
Notes
Dr Fleming died on 1 December 2002
Supplementary
definitions, more detailed results, three extra tables, and three extra
figures are on
bmj.com
We thank Robin A M Forrest, secretary of Westminster Medical School, for
his help in collecting the original data; Naomi Tunnicliffe for her assistance
with this project, Eamonn Ferguson for helpful comments on a draft of the
manuscript, and Rod Rhyss-Jones and Urmila Weller of Imperial College School
of Medicine for their help in tracing doctors. We especially thank the
Westminster Medical School graduates who helped with this study, which was an
intercalated BSc project carried out by ES and PP.
Contributors: PRF initiated the study, and was responsible for collecting
the original data from 1975 to 1982, and ICMcM and PRF collated those data in
1989. ICMcM, ES, and PP designed the present follow up, and ES and PP traced
and contacted the doctors. AK was responsible for data coding and entry.
ICMcM, ES, PP, and AK jointly carried out data analysis. The first draft of
the paper was written by ICMcM, and ES, PP, AK, and PRF contributed to its
revision. ICMcM is guarantor.
Funding: AK was supported by funding from North Thames Postgraduate Medical
and Dental Education.
Competing interests: None declared.
References
1.
McManus IC. Factors affecting likelihood of applicants being
offered a place in medical schools in the United Kingdom in 1996 and 1997:
retrospective study. BMJ 1998;317:
1111-6. [PMC free article] [PubMed]2.
Ferguson E, James D, Madeley L. Factors associated with success in
medical school and in a medical career: systematic review of the literature.
BMJ 2002;324:
952-7. [PMC free article] [PubMed]3. Schmidt FL, Hunter JE. The validity and utility of selection
methods in personnel psychology: practical and theoretical implications of 85
years of research findings. Psychol Bull 1998;124:
262-74.
5. McDonald AS, Newton PE, Whitton C, Higgs SE. A pilot of
aptitude testing for university entrance. London: National
Foundation for Educational Research, 2000.
6. McDonald AS, Newton PE, Whetton C, Benefield P. Aptitude
testing for university entrance: a literature review. London:
National Foundation for Educational Research, 2000.
7.
West PA. Calibre of people recruited to medicine may be too high
for the job [letter]. BMJ 2001;322:
1361. [PMC free article] [PubMed]8. Heim AW. AH5 group test of high-grade
intelligence. Windsor: National Foundation for Educational
research (NFER), 1968.
9. McManus IC, Tunnicliffe N, Fleming PR. The independent effects of
intelligence and educational achievements in predicting final examination
success (abstract). Med Educ 1990;24:
181-4.
10. Costa PT, McCraw RR. Revised NEO personality inventory
and NEO five-factor inventory: professional manual. Odessa, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources, 1992.
11. Matthews G, Deary IJ. Personality traits.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998.
12.
McManus IC, Winder BC, Gordon D. The causal links between stress
and burnout in a longitudinal study of UK doctors.
Lancet 2002;359:
2089-90. [PubMed]13. Peers IS, Johnston M. Influence of learning context on the
relationship between A-level attainment and final degree performance: a
meta-analytic review. Br J Educ Psychol 1994;64:
1-17.
15. Universities UK. Fair enough? Wider access to university
by identifying potential to succeed. London: Universities UK,
2003.
16.
McManus IC, Richards P, Winder BC, Sproston KA. Clinical
experience, performance in final examinations, and learning style in medical
students: prospective study. BMJ 1998;316:
345-50. [PMC free article] [PubMed]17.
McManus IC, Richards P, Winder BC. Intercalated degrees, learning
styles, and career preferences: prospective longitudinal study of UK medical
students. BMJ 1999;319:
542-6. [PMC free article] [PubMed]